Skip to the main content

Original scientific paper

https://doi.org/10.7906/indecs.18.4.2

Working Memory from the Perspective of the Multicomponent Model and Embedded-Processes Model

Anka Slana Ozimič ; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of Ljubljana


Full text: english pdf 479 Kb

page 515-523

downloads: 1.018

cite

Download JATS file


Abstract

In this article, we focus on working memory, the ability to store and actively manipulate information for a short period of time, and present two prominent theoretical frameworks for its study: Baddeley and Hitch’s multicomponent model of working memory and Cowan’s embedded-processes model. The multicomponent model assumes modality specific “slave” components for temporary storage and rehearsal of information and a central executive component that controls the entire system and determines what information enters and leaves the stores. The embedded-processes model, on the other hand, gives a more general description of the working memory system by focusing on its processes. It assumes that attention allocated to representations stored in the long-term memory underlies the short-term maintenance of information. We further describe in more detail how models conceptualize and define working memory, its components, and the processes involved, as well as factors in limiting its capacity. Finally, we describe similarities and differences between the models and present how the components of the models can be mapped to one another and to the brain systems.

Keywords

working memory; multicomponent model; model of embedded-processes; representations; active maintenance

Hrčak ID:

255409

URI

https://hrcak.srce.hr/255409

Publication date:

30.10.2020.

Visits: 2.960 *




INTRODUCTION

Working memory is one of the basic cognitive abilities, crucial for the successful execution of daily tasks such as remembering a telephone number, keeping up with the storyline while reading, solving math problems as well as adjusting to the ever-changing environment when driving a car. In contrast to short-term memory, which assumes simple storage of information for a short period of time [1], working memory refers to both temporary storage as well as active manipulation of information in order to achieve current task goals [2]. The term short-term memory was introduced by Atkinson and Shiffrin [1] in their multi-store model of memory, which assumes that human memory consists of three structural components: the sensory register, the short-term store and the long-term memory store. Information from the environment first enters the sensory register which is modality specific (e.g. vision, hearing) and can hold information for a very brief period of time. If attention is allocated to the sensory stimulation, the information can be transferred into the unitary limited capacity short-term store in which it is present in a readily available state, and if rehearsed it can be further transferred into the essentially unlimited-capacity long-term memory for an indefinite period of time. By popularizing the term “working memory” a few years later, Baddeley and Hitch [3], who found Atkinson and Shiffrin’s [1] model too simplistic, aimed to replace the concept of “short-term” memory by emphasizing its ability to actively manipulate information rather than only passively store it. Active manipulation of temporarily stored information is central to cognition, as it forms the basis for higher cognitive functions such as reasoning, problem-solving and planning. Working memory correlates strongly with fluid intelligence [4] and is often impaired in diseases of the brain [5], its decline, however, is also a signature of healthy aging[6]. As such, understanding the mechanisms of working memory is of key interest in understanding human cognition. One of the key challenges in working memory research is to understand the mechanisms of its highly limited capacity. In early research, the capacity associated with the short-term memory was estimated to 7 ± 2 pieces or “chunks” of information, known as the magical number seven[7]. These chunks of information could be words, letters, digits or units of other types. It was later shown that the capacity estimate heavily depends on the type of information to be memorized (e.g. verbal, visual) as well as the complexity of the stimuli, such as phonological complexity for verbal contents, i.e. the number of phonemes or syllables[8] or complexity of visual stimuli[9]. Therefore, when estimating the capacity, these and other factors affecting memory span must be taken into account, making capacity difficult to be summarized with one specific number valid within different contexts, tasks and information types. Cowan[10], however, proposed that working memory capacity in young adults is about four chunks of information. Though the debate in this field has first centered on the quantification of working memory capacity, the focus has recently shifted to the question of what is the basis for its limited capacity – are the limitations due to limited capacity of modality-specific stores or limitations in attentional processes[11, 12]? A number of models that have evolved within the framework of cognitive psychology, neuroscience and computational modelling attempt to explain its limitations by describing its structures and components as well as their processes. Two of the key theoretical frameworks for studying working memory are Baddeley and Hitch’s[3, 13] multicomponent model of working memory and Cowan’s [4] model of embedded-processes.

THE MULTICOMPONENT MODEL OF WORKING MEMORY

According to Baddeley and Logie[14], working memory “allows humans to comprehend and mentally represent their immediate environment, to retain information about their immediate past experience, to support the acquisition of new knowledge, to solve problems, and to formulate, relate and act on current goals”[14; p.28]. In their multicomponent model of working memory, Baddeley and Hitch [3] describe working memory as a hypothetical system of limited capacity that enables temporary storage and manipulation of information needed to perform a number of cognitive activities. In their view, working memory is a system separate from long-term memory, though tightly connected with it. The model consists of several components: modality-specific temporary “slave” memory systems, which store information and refresh memory traces, and a supervisory system (the central executive) that controls, coordinates and regulates slave memory systems and activates contents from the long-term memory (Fig.1). Initially, unlike Atkinson and Shiffrin’s [1] unitary short-term store, two components for storing information were incorporated into the model [3]: a phonological loop tasked with maintenance of information in phonological form and a visuospatial sketchpad responsible for storing visual and spatial information. Baddeley and Hitch [3] further envisioned that the phonological loop consists of a passive ‘phonological store’ of limited capacity and an active ‘articulatory control process’ for refreshing memory traces and preventing them from temporal decay, which is the primary mechanism of forgetting in working memory. By focusing on visuospatial working memory, Logie[9] upgraded Baddeley and Hitch’s model. Analogous to the phonological loop, he envisaged that visuospatial sketch-pad also consists of a passive store for visual information (visual store) and an active system for refreshing and spatial manipulation of information (inner scribe)[9], although it is to date not entirely clear how visuo-spatial rehearsal is carried out. In addition to the phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketch-pad, the central executive was also included in the original multicomponent model of working memory [3]. Baddeley and Hitch [3] envisioned the central executive as a supervisor, which manages the entire system and manipulates the information in the stores by determining what enters and leaves them. It is tasked with various executive functions such as focusing and switching attention, coordinating the slave systems as well as activating the contents within long-term memory. While slave systems are involved in the temporary storage of information and are of limited memory capacity, the central executive is not involved in information storage and its capacity is limited by attentional resources [2]. A fourth component, the episodic buffer, was added to the model almost 30 years later[13]. It is tasked with storing integrated information of various modalities in the form of short episodes and is tightly connected to long-term memory[13]. The initial assumption was that the storage of bound information depended on the central executive control of attention, but empirical findings confirmed that the episodic buffer – in contrast to the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketch-pad, which contain their own mechanisms for information rehearsal – was a passive structure, which does not need the attention of the central executive to maintain information[15],16], but rather that the central executive is needed to defend relevant contents against disturbing stimuli[16, 17]. In the multicomponent model of working memory representations are established in modality-specific components for short-term storage of information (phonological loop, visuo-spatial sketch-pad, and episodic buffer). Active maintenance of the representations established in buffer stores is enabled through their rehearsal processes (articulatory control process, inner scribe), as well as the central executive processes that control and regulate the information flow in subsidiary systems. In the multicomponent model, the storage of items in the working memory is distinct, thought tightly connected to the contents of long-term memory. In the embedded-processes model, information in the long-term memory can be activated either automatically or voluntarily with the control of central executive processes. When the incoming sensory information is of high salience or when the information is activated by central executive processes it becomes part of the focus of attention, which enables its active maintenance. Note that solid borders denote limited working memory capacity components.

Figure 1. Comparison of the multicomponent and embedded-processes models of working memory.
indecs-18-515-g1.png

EMBEDDED-PROCESSES MODEL

According to Cowan[18] “working memory refers to cognitive processes that retain information in an unusually accessible state, suitable for carrying out any task with a mental component” [18; p. 62]. The idea of Cowan’s embedded-processes model [4], which is probably the most prominent representative of state-based models[19], is to give a more general description of the working memory system by describing the processes it involves rather than to divide its components based on the form of stored information, as in the multicomponent model [2]. Though Cowan [4] acknowledges that visuospatial and verbal memory represent two different systems, he argues that beyond verbal and visuo-spatial representations, other, equally important types of information might be stored in working memory. Cowan’s [4] idea with the embedded-processes model is therefore “to describe a processing model that is exhaustive, in that nothing is left out, even at the cost of the model being vague in places”[4; p.74]. In line with this view, instead of specialized temporary storage systems and a central executive that controls them, the model of embedded-processes sees working memory as a system for controlling attention to the currently activated contents of episodic and semantic long-term memory. Within this perspective, attention allocated to internal representations that are stored either in long-term memory[4,, 20] or established through sensory or motor systems[21,22], forms the basis for short-term maintenance of information. The embedded-processes model is a representative of unitary models of memory, as it assumes that working memory is an active part of long-term memory and identifies broader-to-more specific hierarchically organized components [4]: long-term memory, activated long-term memory and the focus of attention (Fig. 1). Activated long-term memory is a collection of long-term memory representations, that are currently activated and are as such in a particularly accessible state for a limited time. It does not have limited capacity in terms of how many representations can be activated a given moment, but is limited by time and is subject to interference effects. The second component is the capacity-limited focus of attention, which represents a subset of representations in the activated long-term memory. The focus is controlled either voluntarily by central executive processes or involuntary by orienting responses to changes in the environment. Whereas sensory representations can be activated automatically, attention is needed for integration of representations and forming new connections in working memory.

SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MODELS

Although the models seem to differ to a great extent – the components in the multicomponent model are divided based on the properties of stored information (verbal, visuo-spatial, integrated information), while the focus of the embedded-processes model is on the components’ functions – both Baddeley [2] and Cowan [4] see them as essentially complementary[9]. Baddeley[2] argues that “At a superficial level, Cowan’s theories might seem to be totally different from my own. In practice, however, we agree on most issues but differ in our terminology and areas of current focus. /…/ I regard our differences as principally ones of emphasis and terminology”[2; p.20]. Cowan’s [4] view is similar: “Thus the difference between the working memory model of Baddeley (2000) and the embedded processes model is not very large and is probably best viewed as one of the level of analysis: a level of specific phonological, visuospatial and episodic storage properties versus a level of general principles of activated memory and attentional focus”[4; p.84]. The models can be compared in a number of ways and herein we only discuss a few. First, the models can be contrasted in how they see working memory in relation to sensory input and long-term memory. Both the multicomponent model [2] as well as embedded-processes model[18] see sensory input as separate from the contents stored in the working memory[9]. This view is similar to what was proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin [1]. The Multicomponent model assumes that sensory input is first processed by the perceptual systems and can then be transferred, manipulated and stored in the slave components with the help of attention from the central executive, and also by interacting with the long-term memory. Similarly, the embedded-processes model assumes that the sensory input that induces brief sensory afterimages first excites relevant features in the long-term memory, which can then enter the focus of attention either automatically or with the help of executive processes [4]. However, they differ in how they see a connection to long-term memory. The multicomponent model [2] assumes that working memory is distinct, though tightly connected to the contents of long-term memory, whereas the embedded-processes model sees working memory as an activated part of the long-term memory[18]. Second, the models can be compared with regard to the systems and processes they involve. Both propose two systems that are involved in the maintenance of information – one is responsible for establishing and storing representations and the other for their active maintenance. The multicomponent model assumes [2] that representations are established and maintained in the slave components’ stores (phonological store, visual store, episodic buffer), and that their active maintenance is enabled through their rehearsal processes (e.g. articulatory control process, inner scribe), as well as central executive processes that control and regulate the information flow in subsidiary systems[14]. In the embedded-processes model [4] representations are established within the long-term memory system, whereas central executive processes enable their active maintenance in the focus of attention. Though the models are similar in that they both assume two systems are involved in the maintenance of information, it is however not entirely clear whether and how their components can be mapped to one another. In Cowan’s [4] view, his activated long-term memory maps onto Baddeley’s temporary storage systems: “Activated memory was meant to serve the same purpose as Baddeley’s two buffer stores together (phonological and visuospatial), plus any other buffer stores that might be posited in the future.”[4; p.78]. Though no modality-specific buffer systems are specified in Cowan’s [4] model, Cowan does acknowledge that activated portions of long-term memory can relate to distinct buffers. Moreover, he recognizes the role of central executive processes in focusing attention in both models, and links the role of this focus of attention to bind information to Baddeley’s episodic buffer. Similarly, it can be understood that Baddeley [2] also maps his episodic buffer to Cowan’s focus of attention and contents stored in the buffer stores to Cowan’s activated long-term memory: “I see Cowan’s model as principally concerned, in my terminology, with the link between the CE and the episodic buffer. Cowan refers to the material on which his system works as “activated LTM”[2; p.20]. Another important aspect when comparing the two models is how they explain limitations in working memory capacity. In the multicomponent model, the limited capacity of the working memory is viewed as a result of an interaction between the operations of multiple components and subcomponents, and can be understood as an emergent property[9]. While the central executive does not have a limited memory capacity to store information, it is limited by attentional resources. Information storage systems, however, are limited in terms of how much information they can store, which also depends on the complexity of stimuli[9]. Moreover, unless properly refreshed, representations stored in phonological and visual store are subject to temporal decay, and can only hold information for about two seconds. Though the processes for rehearsing information (i.e. articulatory control process for verbal information and inner scribe for visuo-spatial information) have no limit in terms of how many units of information they can refresh, they can only refresh information that is currently present in the buffer stores and as such depend on their capacity. Whereas the main limitation of working memory capacity in the multicomponent model can be ascribed to the buffer store’s capacities, in the embedded-processes model limitations are primarily related to the limited capacity of the focus of attention, as the central executive enables active maintenance in the focus of attention only for a limited number of representations in the activated long-term memory. On the other hand, the activated long-term memory itself has unlimited memory capacity and is limited by temporal and interference effects [4].

MODELS IN RELATION TO BRAIN SYSTEMS

Although the models presented here are intended to give a conceptual description of the structure and processes of working memory and not to map components onto the brain systems, they are consistent with the findings of neurophysiological research showing that posterior and prefrontal areas of the cerebral cortex play different roles in the short-term maintenance of visual-spatial information[23]. Studies show that the posterior areas of the cerebral cortex are involved in the formation and short-term storage of the visual, spatial and verbal representations[24], whereas prefrontal regions control allocation of attention for their active maintenance[25]. The role of prefrontal areas thus resonates with Cowan’s executive processes that control what is in the focus of attention and Baddeley’s rehearsal processes which refresh information from the stores to keep it active, as well as the central executive, which carries out the active work by controlling the contents in the buffer stores. Within Cowan\s model the posterior regions are associated with both activated long-term memory as well as focus of attention [4], whereas in the multicomponent model the posterior regions are associated with contents in component-specific stores, including the episodic buffer [2]. Recent findings from neuroimaging studies [for a review, see 26] show that the posterior brain regions that are involved in temporary storage of information are not unique to working memory, but are also involved in storing long-term memory representations as well as the sensory processing of information[27], 28]. These findings are consistent with the embedded-processes model, which assumes that the focus of attention to currently activated long-term memory representations underlies working memory.

CONCLUSION

As Baddeley [2] states, the idea of the multicomponent model of working memory which also holds true for the embedded-processes model is “a relatively loose theoretical framework rather than a precise model that allows specific predictions.”[2; p.7]. The models must therefore be understood as hypothetical accounts of working memory structure and function. Although the models successfully explain many of the cognitive phenomena associated with the short-term storage of information and its active manipulation, and can be in many respects meaningfully mapped onto the brain function, their purpose is not to explain all of its aspects. They can instead be understood as working platforms that need to be further developed in accordance with empirical findings.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The author acknowledges this article was financially supported by the Slovenian Research Agency project grants J7-5553, J3-9264, and programme P5-0110. This manuscript is an extended version of the article Two approaches to defining and studying working memory presented at the Information Society multiconference held in Ljubljana (7 October – 11 October 2019) on the initiative of the Expert Committee.

References

1 

Atkinson R.C.. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. New York: Academic Press, 1968.

2 

Baddeley A.D. 2012. Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annual Review Psychology, 63, 1 1-29. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

3 

Baddeley A.D.. Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press, 1974.

4 

Cowan N.. Working Memory Capacity. New York: Psychology Press, 2005.

5 

Goldman-Rakic P.S. 1994. Working Memory Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 6, 4 348-357. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/jnp.6.4.348

6 

Park D.C. 2017. Theories of Memory and Aging: A Look at the Past and a Glimpse of the Future. GERONB, 72, 1 82-90. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw066

7 

Miller G.A. 1956. The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63, 2 81-97. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0043158

8 

Service E. 1998. The Effect of Word Length on Immediate Serial Recall Depends on Phonological Complexity, Not Articulatory Duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 2 283-304. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755759

9 

Logie R.H. 2011. The Functional Organization and Capacity Limits of Working Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 4 240-245. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721411415340

10 

Cowan N. 2001. The Magical Number 4 in Short-Term Memory: A Reconsideration of Mental Storage Capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1 87-185. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01003922

11 

Luck S.J. 2013. Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 8 391-400. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.006

12 

Ma W.J. 2014. Changing concepts of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 3 347-356. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3655

13 

Baddeley A. 2000. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 11 417-423. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2

14 

Baddeley A.. Models of working memory Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.

15 

Allen R.J. 2006. Is the binding of visual features in working memory resource-demanding?. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 135, 2 298-313. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.298

16 

Allen R.J. 2012. Feature binding and attention in working memory: A resolution of previous contradictory findings. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 12 2369-2383. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.687384

17 

Schneegans S. 2018. New perspectives on binding in visual working memory. British Journal of Psychology, 110, 2 207-244. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12345

18 

Cowan N.. Models of working memory Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.

19 

D’Esposito M. 2015. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115-142. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031

20 

Oberaurer K. 2009. Design for a Working Memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45-100. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51002-X

21 

Magnussen S. 2000. Low-level memory processes in vision. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 6 247-251. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01569-1

22 

Zaksas D. 2001. Motion Information Is Spatially Localized in a Visual Working-Memory Task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 2 912-921. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.912

23 

Riggall A.C. 2012. The Relationship between Working Memory Storage and Elevated Activity as Measured with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 38 12990-12998. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1892-12.2012

24 

Bettencourt K.C. 2016. Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 1 150-157. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.4174

25 

Eriksson J. 2015. Neurocognitive Architecture of Working Memory. Neuron, 88, 1 33-46. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020

26 

Nee D.E. 2013. A Meta-analysis of Executive Components of Working Memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2 246-282.

27 

Jonides J. 2008. The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193-224. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615

28 

Postle B.R. 2006. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139, 1 23-38. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005


This display is generated from NISO JATS XML with jats-html.xsl. The XSLT engine is libxslt.